Do Developmental Dyscalculic Children have Spatial Biases in Processing Numbers? Evidences from SNARC Effect
- SNARC effect,
- Developmental dyscalculia,
- internal number line
Abstract
Developmental dyscalculia (DD) is a specific learning disability affecting the normal acquisition of arithmetic skills, it is hypothesized that they may be lacking ability to represent and manipulate numerical magnitude nonverbally on an internal number line. Hence the present study wasattempted with the aim of observing spatial biases if any in children with developmental dyscalculia using spatial numerical association of response codes (SNARC) paradigm. Participants consistedof 12 children with developmental dyscalculia and thirty typically developing children in the age range of 9-10 years. Each participant received four white pages containing written instructions on the first page; and the other 2 pages contained 16 strings of digits and the other with 16 lines. All participants were instructed to bisect each stimulus in the middle. The distance between a bisection mark and both ends of a line was determined to the nearest millimeter, yielding a left and right interval for each stimulus. The difference score (left interval minus right interval) yielded a negative value when performance was biased to the left and a positive value when performance was biased to the right. The results revealed no evidence of SNARC effect in both the groups of children i.e., normal controls exhibited a right handed bias for both small and large numbers and left handed bias for lines whereas children with developmental dyscalculia exhibited a left sided bias for both lines and numbers. In the present study, we investigated spatial biases if any while processing numbers in children with developmental dyscalculia. The results revealed no evidence of SNARC effect in both the groups of children. The present observations, if substantiated by further research, may be useful for the diagnosis of number comprehension skills in children with developmental dyscalculia
References
Berch, D.B., Foley, E.J., Hill, R.J., & Ryan, P.M. (1999). Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 286-308.
Brysbaert, M. (1995). Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 434-452.
Butterworth, B. (2005). Developmental Dyscalculia. In: Campbell, J.D. The Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. New York: Psychology Press. 455-469.
Butterworth, B. (2005). The development of arithmetical abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 318.
Dehaene, S. (1989). The psychophysics of numerical comparison: A reexamination of apparently incompatible data. Perception & Psychophysics, 45, 557-566.
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S. & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 122, 371-396.
Dehaene, S. (1997). The Number Sense. How the Mind Creates Mathematics. New York: Oxford University Press.
Dehaene, S. (1989). The psychophysics of numerical comparison: A re-examination of apparently incompatible data. Perception & Psychophysics, 45, 557-566.
Dehaene, S. (2001). Précise of the number sense. Mind & Language, 16, 16-36.
Dobler, V., Manly, T., Atkinson, J., Wilson, B. A., Ioannou, K., & Robertson, I. H. (2001). Interaction of hand use and spatial selective attention in children. Neuropsychologica, 39, 1055-1064.
Fias, W. (2001). Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Psychological Research, 65, 250- 259.
Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & d'Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition,2, 95-110.
Fischer, M.H. (2000).Word centre is misperceived. Perception, 29, 337-354.
Fischer, M. H. (2001). Number processing induces spatial performance biases. Neurology, 57, 822- 826.
Galton, F. (1880a). Visualized numerals. Nature, 21,252-256.
Galton, F. (1880b). Visualized numerals. Nature, 21,494-495.
Geary, D. C., & Hoard, M. K. (2001). Neumerical and arithmetical deficits in learning disabled children: Relation to dyscalculia and dyslexia. Aphasiology,15, 635-647.
Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C.R., (2004). Language and the origin of numerical concepts. Science, 306,441-443.
Giaquinto, M. (2001). What cognitive systems underlie arithmetical abilities? Mind Language, 16, 56-68
Houde, O., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2003). Neural foundations of logical and mathematical cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 507-514.
Landerl, K., Bevan, A., & Butterworth, B. (2004). Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: a study of 89-year-old students. Cognition, 93, 99-125.
Moyer, R.S., & Landauer, T.K. (1967). Time required for judgments of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519-1520.
Noel, M. P. (2001). Numerical cognition. In R. Brenda (Ed.), The handbook of cognitive neuropsychology: What deficits reveal about the human mind, 495518, London: Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis.
Purushothama, G. (1992). Items of reading test and construction of a diagnostic reading test in kannada. Journal of All India Institute of Speech & Hearing, 23, 15-29.
Ramaa, S. (1994). Arithmatic diagnostic test for primary school children. An Unpublished doctoralthesis submitted to the regional institute foreducation, University of Mysore, Mysore.
Ratinckx, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2002). Interhemispheric Stroop-like interference in number comparison: Evidence for strong interhemispheric integration of semantic number information. Neuropsychology, 16, 217-229.
Reynvoet, B., & Brysbaert, M. (1999). Single-digit and two-digit Arabic numerals address the same semantic number line. Cognition, 72, 191-201.Houde, O., Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2003).Neural foundations of logical and mathematical cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 507- 514.
Rotzer, S., Loenneker, T., Kucian, K., Martin, E., Kalver, P., & Von Aster, M. (2009) Dysfunctional neural network of spatial working memory contributes to developmental dyscalculia, Neuropsychologia, 22, 224-229.
Rubinsten, O., Henik, A. (2005). Automatic activation of internal magnitudes: a study of developmental dyscalculia. Neuropsychology, 19, 641-648.
Seron, X., Pesenti, M., Noel, M.P., Deloche, G., & Cornet, J.A. (1992). Images of numbers, or 'when 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue.' Cognition, 44, 159-196.
Siegler, R.S.,& Opfer, J.E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychological Science,14, 237-242.
Siegler, R.S., & Booth, J.L., (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Development, 75, 428-444.
Shaki, S., & Petrusic, W. M. (2005). On the mental representation of negative numbers: Context dependent SNARK effects with comparative judgements. Psychonomic bulletin & Review, 12, 931-937
Vallesi, A., Binns, M. A., & Shallice, T. (2008). An Effect of spatial-temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition, 107, 501-527.
Van Galen, M. S., & Reitsma, P. (2008). Developing access number magnitude: A study of the SNARC effect in 7- to 9-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 536-562.
Von Aster, M. G., & Shalev, R. S. (2007). Number development and developmental dyscalculia. Developmental medicine and child neurology, 49,868-873.